Societies Evolve

000This introductory note from Philosophical Transactions brought together papers presented at a Discussion Meeting in January 2009 where 15 scientists were invited to review important issues relevant to our understanding of the evolution of society in animals and humans. Their aim was to explore similarities and contrasts in evolutionary mechanisms in different groups of organisms and the relevance of studies of animal societies to humans. I am reproducing it here in full without permission:
. 2009 Nov 12; 364(1533): 3127–3133.

***

The Evolution of Society

Although the social mechanisms responsible for the development and maintenance of societies in animals and man have fascinated and intrigued philosophers and scientists since classical times, the first systematic consideration of their evolution appears in the Origin of species (). Much of Darwin’s thinking about the evolution of societies in animals and humans has a distinctly modern feel about it and he commonly anticipates theoretical developments that only occurred 100 years later. Although he did not confront the problem of altruistic behaviour directly, he was aware of the challenge to his theory posed by the evolution of sterile castes in some social insects (). In Chapter VIII of the ‘Origin of species’, he describes how he thought, at first, that this was fatal to his whole theory of natural selection. Then, in a paragraph that presages Hamilton’s subsequent extension of evolutionary theory, he describes how he realised that ‘the problem is lessened, or, as I believe, disappears, when it is remembered that selection may be applied to the family, as well as to the individual, and may thus gain the desired end.’ (, p. 230).
In The descent of man (1871), Darwin turned to the evolution of human societies. In Chapter VI, he stresses the contrast between humans and other animals ‘I fully subscribe to the judgement of those writers who maintain that of all the differences between man and the lesser animals, the moral sense or conscience is by far the most important’ (The descent of man, p. 97). He then goes on to argue that the evolution of mutual assistance and the moral senses in humans and other animals are maintained by benefits shared by members of cooperative groups, a suggestion that clearly parallels modern theories of social evolution (). He goes on to point out that many animals live in groups and cooperate with each other and describes how ‘wolves and some other beasts of prey hunt in packs, and aid one another in attacking their victims’, how ‘pelicans fish in concert’ and ‘social animals mutually defend each other’. He describes how vervet monkeys stretch out and groom each others coats and ends by telling a story illustrating the benefits of cooperation:
‘an eagle seizes a young Cercopithecus, which by clinging to a branch, was not at once carried off; it cried loudly for assistance, upon which the other members of the troop, with much uproar, rushed to the rescue, surrounded the eagle, and pulled out so many feathers, that he no longer thought of his prey, but only how to escape. This eagle as Brehm [the source of the story] remarks, assuredly would never again attack a single monkey of a troop’ (p. 101, 102).
For nearly a 100 years from Darwin’s death, scientific attention was focussed on mechanistic and developmental questions rather than functional ones and Darwin’s interest in social evolution and his holistic view of biological adaptation were eclipsed by the growth of other biological subdisciplines. A continuing interest in social behaviour was maintained though the research and writings of naturalists like Henri Fabre, Eugene Marais, the Keatons, Edmund Selous and Eliot Howard. However, although they were experienced naturalists and observers, they lacked Darwin’s theoretical structure, his compelling interest in principles and his readiness to confront exceptions and difficulties.
Not until the late 1930s did a substantial number of professional biologists start to work on the social behaviour of animals. They fell into three main groups. First, there were the founding fathers of animal behaviour, including Julian Huxley (1934, 1938), Konrad Lorenz (1927, 1931, 1935), Niko Tinbergen (1931, 1935, 1936, 1937), Karl von Frisch (1938), Frank Fraser Darling (1937, 1938), Solly Zuckerman (1929, 1932) and Clarence Ray Carpenter (1934, 1935, 1940). Their primary focus was usually on questions concerning the control and development of behaviour, though their research sometimes encompassed functional or comparative aspects of reproductive behaviour. Second, there were a number of animal ecologists, including David Lack (1932, 1933, 1935, 1939, 1943) and A. F. Skutch (1935, 1945, 1960) whose primary interests were the regulation of population density and the evolution of reproductive parameters, including egg size and clutch size. And third, there were the population geneticists, including Ronald Fisher (1930) and J. B. S. Haldane (1932) and later,  whose principal focus was on the operation of natural selection and the evolution of genetic systems, but whose interests also encompassed the evolution of life histories and social behaviour. Unlike the first two groups, they were well aware of the problems raised by social and altruistic behaviour, though these were tangential to their main interest and usually attracted only passing comments.
These three distinct lines of thinking persisted into the 1950s and they were still largely separate by the end of that decade. For example, neither of Niko Tinbergen’s two synthetic books, Social behaviour in animals (1953) and The study of instinct (1955) cite either Darwin or Fisher. However, by 1960, both theoretical and empirical research was beginning to turn to topics that overlapped all three areas of interest. Comparative studies of social behaviour (especially studies of birds by Gordon Orians (1961, 1962) and John Crook (1962, 1964, 1965) revealed that the size and structure of social groups and the form of mating systems were closely related to variation in habitat and diet, stimulating interest in functional interpretations of social behaviour and providing detailed examples both of selfish and altruistic behaviour. In addition, research on population dynamics in birds showed that territoriality played an important part in regulating population density, focusing the interests of ecologists and ethologists on its evolution and function (Lack ). Finally, the development of a theoretical framework accounting for the evolution of life histories () led to a growing interest in the adaptive significance of apparently altruistic behaviour.
Two specific developments acted as catalysts for the rapid changes that followed. The first was the publication of Wynne Edwards’ monumental book Animal dispersion in relation to social behaviour (1962). Wynne Edwards claimed that many animals adaptively limited their numbers in advance of resource shortage to improve the probability that the group or population would survive. Group displays had evolved, he suggested, to allow their members to assess population density and to adjust their reproductive output accordingly. Other aspects of social behaviour, including territoriality and dominance hierarchies, were closely involved in the regulation of animal numbers and had evolved for this purpose. Wynne Edwards’ theory was directly contrary both to Darwin’s persistent emphasis on individual variation in reproduction as the keystone of evolution as well as to the perception of many ecologists that animal populations were limited directly by the availability of resources (Lack ) so neither population geneticists nor ecologists could ignore the challenge. The general application of Wynne Edwards’ theory was attacked and refuted (; Williams ,) and the ensuing controversy drew attention to the fact that many functional explanations of social and reproductive behaviour relied on putative benefits to groups or populations. This eventually led to a critical revaluation of many of these ideas, culminating in G. C. Williams’ influential review of adaptation (1966).
The second development was the explanation of altruism and sterility in Hymenoptera by W. D. Hamilton. In 1963, Hamilton published a brief paper arguing that altruism could evolve if it increased the fitness of relatives and, the following year, introduced the concept of inclusive fitness to account for the evolution of worker sterility in Hymenoptera and of alarm calls in vertebrates (). Subsequently,  named Hamilton’s process ‘kin selection’ to distinguish it from group selection and used it to produce a formal model of the evolution of alarm calls (). In contrast to many of his contemporaries working on the evolution of vertebrate-breeding systems (see above), Hamilton’s thinking owed much to Fisher. He describes how his interest in the evolution of animal societies and altruism.
‘began for me while I was an undergraduate reading natural sciences at the University of Cambridge in 1958. I discovered R. A. Fisher’s The genetical theory of natural selection in the St John’s College Library and immediately realised that this was the key to the understanding of evolution that I had long wanted. I became a Fisher freak and neglected whole courses in my efforts to grasp the book’s extremely compressed style and reasoning. I quickly noticed, however, that Fisher’s arguments implied a basically different interpretation of adaptation from what I was hearing from most of my lecturers and reading in other books. Was adaptation mainly for the benefit of species (the lecturers’ view) or for the benefit of individuals (Fisher’s view)? Clearly it was Fisher who had thought out his Darwinism properly; where interpretations differed, therefore, he must be right—but were the others always wrong? I started on what seemed the key theme in this puzzle—altruism. Did it exist? Could one evolve it in a model? (, p. 15)
Hamilton’s theory of kin selection () provided the basis for adaptive interpretations of many forms of altruistic and cooperative behaviour. However, there were some types of cooperation that could not be explained in this way. In particular, why should members of different species (who could not possibly be closely related) cooperate with each other? And why should unrelated conspecifics sometimes assist each other? One possible explanation was that, as Darwin had suggested, cooperating individuals gained shared mutualistic benefits but explanations of this kind smacked of group selection and had difficulty in explaining why cooperation was not replaced by cheating strategies. An alternative explanation of apparently altruistic actions involving unrelated individuals was produced by R. L. Trivers in 1971. Trivers argued that if individuals assisted each other in turn and the costs of assistance were relatively low to donors while the benefits were high to recipients, reciprocal assistance (reciprocity) could evolve among individuals that were unrelated to each other. Cheats (individuals who accepted favours but did not return them) might initially be at an advantage but selection would subsequently favour individuals that discriminated against them and cooperated selectively with individuals that had assisted them in the past. This form of cooperation was originally referred to as ‘reciprocal’ altruism but this can lead to confusion since cooperation of this kind is mutually beneficial in the long term rather than altruistic ().
The theoretical basis of much of our current understanding of the evolution of breeding systems was laid during the decade following the publication of Hamilton’s theory of kin selection ().  ‘selfish herd’ theory showed that sociality itself could confer benefits to individuals without benefiting the entire group if the chance that an individual would be selected by a predator was diluted as the number of individuals close to it increased. He subsequently examined the conditions favouring selfish and spiteful behaviour within social groups (Hamilton ).  and  introduced game theory models to explain why competitors did not always seek to maximize damage to their opponents and to account for ‘ritualization’ of aggressive behaviour. Trivers (1974) explored conflicts of interest between parents and their offspring arguing that, in sexually reproducing organisms, the genetic interests of parents will commonly differ from those of their progeny, who should often favour higher levels of parental expenditure higher than their parents’ optima. Darwin’s writing on sexual selection was re-examined and extended. Trivers (1972) argued that the reasons why males typically compete more intensely for mates than do females was linked to their lower expenditure on progeny, coining the term ‘parental investment’ to cover all forms of parental expenditure associated with rearing offspring. Fisher’s (1930) explanation of the equality of most vertebrate sex ratios was re-assessed and Hamilton (1967) showed that the strongly female-biased sex ratios could be favoured where competing males were close relatives. Trivers & Willard (1973) argued that, in sexually dimorphic vertebrates where sons were more costly to rear than daughters, females who conceived sons but could not afford to rear them should prematurely terminate investment in their progeny—and suggested that this might account for the common trend for mortality to be higher in juvenile males than females. Adaptive explanations of life histories developed in a less dramatic fashion. A number of important reviews laid the basis of what is now known as life-history theory (). Over the same period, empirical studies of animal breeding systems began to proliferate, focusing more and more on issues of theoretical interest ().
This period of rapid development of theory culminated in a second monumental book. E. O. Wilson’s Sociobiology, the new synthesis () contained relatively few new developments but provided comprehensive reviews of relevant areas of population genetics, demography, life-history theory and animal behaviour. Wilson stressed their inter-relatedness and defined a new sub-discipline, sociobiology, whose principal goal should be ‘an ability to predict features of social organization from a knowledge of population parameters combined with information on the behavioural constraints imposed by the genetic constitution of the species’. He argued that an understanding of the evolution of population parameters (including life history variables) should be one of the principal aims of evolutionary ecology and population biology and predicted that, by the year 2000, sociobiology and behavioural ecology would have become closely allied with population biology and genetics, while traditional ethology and comparative psychology would have been progressively integrated with neurophysiology. The first component of his prediction came about more rapidly than he had anticipated for the theoretical issues raised by the papers of Hamilton, Trivers and Maynard Smith rapidly became the focus of attention in behavioural biology and soon led to the development of an integrated conceptual framework for explaining the ecology and evolution of social behaviour ().
After the publication of Wilson’s (1975) review, research on social evolution expanded rapidly. The ideas of Hamilton, Trivers, Maynard Smith and Parker were explored, extended and tested (; Krebs & Davies ). A combination of theoretical and empirical studies investigated the contrasts and parallels between cooperative and competitive interactions at different levels—between genes on the same chromosome, between cells and groups of cells, between individuals, families, groups and populations (). Building on research on the development of social relationships (Hinde ), research on social vertebrates (and primates in particular) explored the development and maintenance of relationships and the ways by which individuals attempt to manipulate the behaviour of others to their own advantage (Clutton-Brock & Parker ,), and the tactics they use to resolve conflicts (). New fields of research developed round the evolution of signalling systems (), cooperation between non-relatives (), the evolution of cooperative breeding (), the extent and causes of reproductive suppression (Vehrencamp ,) and the resolution of conflicts within social groups (). The adaptive significance of life-history parameters was explored and examined and new theories were developed to account for variation in fecundity (), mate choice (), sex allocation (), parental care (; Godfray ,) and longevity (). Following the development of genetic techniques capable of identifying paternity () it soon came to be appreciated that competition between males extended beyond mating (). These empirical advances were associated with theoretical developments that clarified the links between inclusive fitness and other branches of evolutionary theory, including population and quantitative genetics (), making it easier to develop more general models () and allowing the biology to lead the maths, rather than vice versa.
Over the same period, long-term studies of recognizable individuals provided novel insights into animal societies as well as the information necessary to explore new questions (). In addition, the increasing range of societies that had been examined in detail generated reviews of social behaviour and breeding systems in insects (), fishes (), birds () and non-human mammals (; Smuts ,). Quantitative comparisons of interspecific data were used to test evolutionary hypotheses and to explore relationships between social behaviour, life histories and ecological parameters (; Clutton-Brock & Harvey ). The framework of evolutionary thinking was also extended to the analysis of human behaviour, relationships and societies (). Theoretical studies investigated the evolution of the unusual characteristics of humans, including the development of culturally acquired adaptations (). In parallel, empirical studies of tribal societies generated quantative data on behaviour, energetics, life-history parameters and demography that could be used to test ecological and evolutionary theories and predictions ().
Today, we understand more about the evolution of society than Darwin did. Nevertheless, the field of social evolution continues to develop rapidly and there are still many unresolved problems and many contentious areas. There is an ongoing debate as to whether social systems should be regarded as superorganisms with adaptive characteristics of their own or whether they are best interpreted as byproducts of the adaptive strategies of individuals (). There is still disagreement about the distinction between kin selection and group selection as well as about the relative importance of selection operating at different levels () and the relative importance of mutualism, reciprocity and coercion in maintaining cooperative behaviour is contentious (). Evolutionary explanations of sex differences have recently been the target of criticism () and some would even like to see the theory of sexual selection abandoned altogether (). Contrasting models of variation in reproductive skew and the proximate mechanisms responsible for reproductive suppression in cooperative societies are still widely debated (Vehrencamp ,). Finally, there is little agreement over the origins of human society; the sequence in which human characteristics developed or the relevance of studies of animal societies to understanding those of humans ().

REFERENCES

  • Alexander R. D. 1991The evolution of eusociality. In The biology of the naked mole-rat (eds Sherman P. W., Jarvis J. U. M., Alexander R. D., editors. ), pp. 3–44 Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press []
  • Aureli F., de Waal F. B. M. 2000Natural conflict resolution San Francisco, CA: University of California Press []
  • Axelrod R. 1984The evolution of cooperation New York, NY: Basic Books []
  • Barrett L., Dunbar R., Lycett J. E. 2002Human evolutionary psychology Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press []
  • Bergmuller R., Bshary R., Johnstone R. A., Russell A. F. 2007aIntegrating cooperative breeding and cooperation theoryBehav. Process. 76, 61–72 (doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2007.07.001) [PubMed[]
  • Betzig L., Borgerhoff Mulder M., Turkes P., editors. (eds) 1988Human reproductive behaviour Cambridge, UK: University Press []
  • Birkhead T. R., Møller A. P. 1992Sperm competitions in birds London, UK: Academic Press []
  • Boomsma J. J., Grafen A. 1990Intraspecific variation in ant sex ratios and the Trivers–Hare hypothesisEvolution 44, 1026–1034 (doi:10.2307/2409564) [PubMed[]
  • Borgerhoff Mulder M. 1988Reproductive success in three Kipsigi cohorts. In Reproductive success (ed. Clutton-Brock T. H., editor. ), pp. 419–435 Chicago, IL: University Press []
  • Bourke A. F. G. 1999Colony size, social complexity and reproductive conflict in social insectsJ. Evol. Biol. 12, 245–257 (doi:10.1046/j.1420-9101.1999.00028.x[]
  • Bourke A. F. G. 2007Kin selection and the evolutionary theory of agingAnn. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 38, 103–128 []
  • Bourke A. F. G., Franks N. R. 1995Social evolution in ants Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press []
  • Boyd R., Richerson P. J. 1996Why culture is common, but cultural evolution is rareProc. Br. Acad. 88, 77–93 []
  • Brown J. L. 1987Helping and communal breeding in birds Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press []
  • Bull J. J. 1983Evolution of sex determining mechanisms Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings []
  • Charlesworth B. 1980Evolution in age-structured populations Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press []
  • Charnov E. L. 1982The theory of sex allocation Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press []
  • Choe J. L., Crespi B. J., editors. (eds) 1997Mating systems in insects and arachnids Cambridge, UK: University Press []
  • Clutton-Brock T. H. 1974Primate social organisation and ecologyNature 250, 539–542 (doi:10.1038/250539a0[]
  • Clutton-Brock T. H. 1991The evolution of parental care Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press []
  • Clutton-Brock T. H. 1998Reproductive skew: disentangling concessions from control. A reply to Emlen and ReeveTrends Ecol. Evol. 13, 459 (doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01470-0) [PubMed[]
  • Clutton-Brock T. H. 2002Breeding together: kin selection and mutualism in cooperative vertebratesScience 296, 69–72 (doi:10.1126/science.296.5565.69) [PubMed[]
  • Clutton-Brock T. Cooperation between non-kin: reciprocity, mutualism or manipulation? Nature In press. [PubMed[]
  • Clutton-Brock T. H., Harvey P. H. 1978Mammals, resources and reproductive strategiesNature 273, 191–195 (doi:10.1038/273191a0) [PubMed[]
  • Clutton-Brock T. H., Harvey P. H. 1979Comparison and adaptationProc. R. Soc. Lond. B 205, 547–565 (doi:10.1098/rspb.1979.0084) [PubMed[]
  • Clutton-Brock T. H., Parker G. A. 1995aPunishment in animal societiesNature 373, 209–216 (doi:10.1038/373209a0) [PubMed[]
  • Clutton-Brock T. H., Parker G. A. 1995bSexual coercion in animal societiesAnim. Behav. 49, 1345–1365 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1995.0166[]
  • Clutton-Brock T. H., Guinness F. E., Albon S. D. 1982Red deer: the behaviour and ecology of two sexes Edinburgh, UK: University Press []
  • Clutton-Brock T. H., et al. 2001bCooperation, conflict and concession in meerkat groupsScience 291, 478–481 (doi:10.1126/science.291.5503.478) [PubMed[]
  • Cole L. C. 1954The population consequences of life history phenomenaQuart. Rev. Biol. 29, 103–137 [PubMed[]
  • Creel S., Creel N. M. 2001The African wild dog: behavior, ecology and conservation Princeton, NJ: University Press []
  • Daly M., Wilson M. 1983Sex, evolution and behavior Boston, MA: Willard Grant Press []
  • Darwin C. 1859/1958The origin of species New York, NY: The Modern Library []
  • de Waal F. B. M. 1993Primate social conflict (eds Mason W. A., Mendoza S. P., editors. ). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press []
  • Dugatkin L. A. 1997Cooperation among animals: an evolutionary perspective Oxford, UK: University Press []
  • Frank S. A. 1986Hierarchical selection theory and sex ratios. I. General solutions for structured populationsTheoret. Popul. Biol. 29, 312–342 (doi:10.1016/0040-5809(86)90013-4) [PubMed[]
  • Frank S. A. 1990Sex allocation theory for birds and mammalsAnn. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 21, 13–56 (doi:10.1146/annurev.es.21.110190.000305[]
  • Frank S. A. 1998Foundations of social evolution Princeton, NJ: University Press []
  • Gadgil M., Bossert W. H. 1970Life historical consequences of natural selectionAm. Nat. 104, 1–24 (doi:10.1086/282637[]
  • Gardner A., West S. A., Barton N. H. 2007The relation between multilocus population genetics and social evolution theoryAm. Nat. 169, 207–226 (doi:10.1086/510602) [PubMed[]
  • Gittleman J. L. 1989Carnivore group living: comparative trends. In Carnivore behaviour, ecology and evolution (ed. Gittleman J. L., editor. ), pp. 183–207 Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press []
  • Godfray H. C. J. 1995aEvolutionary theory of parent–offspring conflictNature 376, 133–138 (doi:10.1038/376133a0) [PubMed[]
  • Godfray H. C. J. 1995bSignaling of need between parents and young: parent–offspring conflict and sibling rivalryAm. Nat. 146, 1–24 (doi:10.1086/285784[]
  • Gowaty P. A. 2004Sex roles, contests for the control of reproduction and sexual selection. In Sexual selection in primates (ed. Kappeler P., editor. ), pp. 163–221 Cambridge, UK: University Press []
  • Grafen A. 1985A geometric view of relatednessOxford Surv. Evol. Biol. 2, 28–29 []
  • Grafen A. 1990Biological signals as handicapsJ. Theoret. Biol. 144, 517–546 (doi:10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80088-8) [PubMed[]
  • Hamilton W. D. 1963The evolution of altruistic behaviorAm. Nat. 97, 354–356 (doi:10.1086/497114[]
  • Hamilton W. D. 1964The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. IIJ. Theoret. Biol. 7, 1–52 (doi:10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4) [PubMed[]
  • Hamilton W. D. 1971Geometry for the selfish herdJ. Theoret. Biol. 31, 295–311 (doi:10.1016/0022-5193(71)90189-5) [PubMed[]
  • Hamilton W. D. 1972Altruism and related phenomena, mainly in social insectsAnn. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 3, 193–232 (doi:10.1146/annurev.es.03.110172.001205[]
  • Hamilton W. D. 1988The genetical theory of social behaviour 1. Citation classic in Current Contents No. 40, p. 16. [J. Theor. Biol.7, 1–16]. []
  • Harvey P. H., Pagel M. D. 1991The comparative method in evolutionary biology Oxford, UK: University Press []
  • Hill K., Hurtado M. A. 1996Ache life history New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter []
  • Hinde R. A. 1974Biological bases of human social behaviour New York, NY: McGraw Hill []
  • Hinde R. A. 1983Primate social relationships Oxford, UK: Blackwells []
  • Hoekstra R. 2003Power to the genome: who suppresses the outlaw? In Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation (ed. Hammersteiin P., editor. ), pp. 257–270 Dahlem Workshop Reports series Cambridge, MA: MIT Press []
  • Hoogland J. L. 1995The black-tailed prairie dog: social life of a burrowing mammal Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press []
  • Hrdy S. B. 2009Mothers and others Harvard, USA: Belknap Press []
  • Jarman P. J. 1974The social organisation of antelope in relation to their ecologyBehaviour 48, 215–267 (doi:10.1163/156853974X00345[]
  • Jeffreys A. J., Wilson V., Thein S. L. 1985Hypervariable ‘minisatellite’ regions in human DNANature 314, 67–73 (doi:10.1038/314067a0) [PubMed[]
  • Koenig W., Dickinson J., editors. (eds) 2004Ecology and evolution of cooperative breeding in birds Cambridge, UK: University Press []
  • Koenig W. D., Mumme R. L. 1987Population ecology of the cooperatively breeding acorn woodpecker Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press []
  • Krebs J. R., Davies N. B. 1978Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific Publications []
  • Krebs J. R., Davies N. B. 1984Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific Publications []
  • Krebs J. R., Davies N. B. 1991Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach Oxford, UK: Blackwell []
  • Krebs J. R., Davies N. B. 1997An introduction to behavioural ecology, 4th edn Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific []
  • Lack D. 1954The natural regulation of animal numbers Oxford, UK: University Press []
  • Lack D. 1966Population studies of birds Oxford, UK: University Press []
  • Lack D. 1968Ecological adaptation for breeding in birds London, UK: Methuen []
  • Lande R. 1980Sexual dimorphism, sexual selection and adaptation in polygenic charactersEvolution 34, 292–305 (doi:10.2307/2407393) [PubMed[]
  • Lande R., Arnold S. J. 1983The measurement of selection on correlated charactersEvolution 37, 1210–1226 (doi:10.2307/2408842) [PubMed[]
  • Magrath R. A., Johnstone R. A., Heinsohn R. G. 2004Reproductive skew. In Ecology and evolution of cooperative breeding in birds (eds Koenig W., Dickinson J., editors. ), pp. 157–176 Cambridge, UK: University Press []
  • Mann J., Connor R. C., Tyack P. L., Whitehead H., editors. (eds) 2000Celacean societies Chicago, IL: University Press []
  • Maynard Smith J. 1964Group selection and kin selectionNature 201, 1145–1147 (doi:10.1038/2011145a0[]
  • Maynard Smith J. 1965The evolution of alarm callsAm. Nat. 99, 59–63 []
  • Maynard Smith J. 1974The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflictsJ. Theor. Biol. 47, 209–221 [PubMed[]
  • Maynard Smith J., Szathmary E. 1995The major transitions in evolution New York, NY: Freeman []
  • Medawar P. B. 1952An unsolved problem of biology London, UK: H.K. Lewis & Co []
  • Michod R. E., Roze D. 2001Cooperation and conflict in the evolution of multicellularityHeredity 86, 1–7 (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2540.2001.00808.x) [PubMed[]
  • Okasha S. 2006Evolution and the levels of selection Oxford, UK: University Press []
  • Parker G. A. 1974Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviourJ. Theor. Biol. 47, 223–243 [PubMed[]
  • Pianka E. R. 1974Evolutionary ecology New York, NY: Harper and Row []
  • Queller D. C. 1992Quantitative genetics; inclusive fitness and group selectionAm. Nat. 139, 540–558 (doi:10.1086/285343[]
  • Ratnieks F. L. W. 1988Reproductive harmony via mutual policing by workers in eusocial HymenopteraAm. Nat. 132, 217–236 (doi:10.1086/284846[]
  • Ratnieks F. L. W., Foster K. R., Wenseleers T. 2006Conflict resolution in insect societiesAnn. Rev. Entomol. 51, 581–608 (doi:10.1146/annurev.ento.51.110104.151003) [PubMed[]
  • Reeve H. K., Keller L. 1995Partitioning of reproduction in mother–daughter versus sibling associations: a test of optimal skew theoryAm. Nat. 145, 119–132 (doi:10.1086/285731[]
  • Rodseth L., Wrangham R. W., Harrigan A., Smuts B. B. 1991The human community as a primate societyCurr. Anthropol. 32, 221–254 (doi:10.1086/203952[]
  • Roughgarden J. 2009. In The genial gene: deconstructing Darwinian selfishness San Francisco, CA: University of California Press []
  • Roughgarden J., Oishi M., Akcay E. 2006Reproductive social behavior: cooperative games to replace sexual selectionScience 311, 965–969 (doi:10.1126/science.1110105) [PubMed[]
  • Rousset F., Ronce O. 2004Indirect fitness for traits affecting metapopulation demographyTheoret. Popul. Biol. 65, 127–141 (doi:10.1016/j.tpb.2003.09.003) [PubMed[]
  • Sachs J. L., Mueller U. G., Wilcox T. P., Bull J. J. 2004The evolution of cooperationQuart. Rev. Biol. 79, 135–160 [PubMed[]
  • Smuts B. B. 1986aSexual competition and mate choice. In Primate societies (eds Smuts B. B., Cheney D. L., Seyfarth R. M., Wrangham W. R., Struhsaker T. T., editors. ). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press []
  • Smuts B. B. 1986bGender, aggression and influence. In Primate societies (eds Smuts B. B., Cheney D. L., Seyfarth R. M., Wrangham W. R., Struhsaker T. T., editors. ). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press []
  • Stearns S. C. 1976Life-history tactics: a review of the ideasQuart. Rev. Biol. 51, 3–48 [PubMed[]
  • Tang-Martinez Z., Ryder T. B. 2005The problem with paradigms: Bateman’s world view as a case studyIntegr. Comp. Biol. 45, 821–830 (doi:10.1093/icb/45.5.821) [PubMed[]
  • Taylor P. D. 1990Allele-frequency change in a class structured populationAm. Nat. 135, 95–106 (doi:10.1086/285034[]
  • Taylor P. D. 1996Inclusive fitness arguments in genetic models of behaviourJ. Math. Biol. 34, 654–674 (doi:10.1007/BF02409753) [PubMed[]
  • Taylor P. D., Frank S. A. 1996How to make a kin selection modelJ. Theoret. Biol. 180, 27–37 (doi:10.1006/jtbi.1996.0075) [PubMed[]
  • Thresher R. E. 1984Reproduction in reef fishes Neptune City, NJ: T. F. H. Publications []
  • Trivers R. L., Hare H. 1976Haplo-diploidy and the evolution of the social insectsScience 191, 249–263 (doi:10.1126/science.1108197) [PubMed[]
  • Vehrencamp S. L. 1983aA model for the evolution of despotic versus egalitarian societiesAnim. Behav. 31, 667–682 (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80222-X[]
  • Vehrencamp S. L. 1983bOptimal degree of skew in cooperative societiesAm. Zool. 23, 327–335 []
  • West S. A. 2009Sex allocation Princeton, NJ: University Press []
  • West S. A., Griffin A. S., Gardner A. 2007Social semantics: altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selectionJ. Evol. Biol. 20, 415–432 (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.x) [PubMed[]
  • West S. A., Griffin A. S., Gardner A. 2008Social semantics: how useful has group selection been? J. Evol. Biol. 21, 374–385 []
  • West S. A., Mouden C. E., Gardner A. In press Social evolution theory and its application to the evolution of cooperation in humans[]
  • Whiten A., Byrne R. W. 1997Machiavellian Intelligence II Cambridge, UK: University Press []
  • Williams G. C. 1957Pleiotropy, natural selection, and the evolution of senescenceEvolution 11, 398–411 (doi:10.2307/2406060[]
  • Williams G. C. 1966aAdaptation and natural selection: a critique of some current evolutionary thought Princeton, NJ: University Press []
  • Williams G. C. 1966bNatural selection, the costs of reproduction, and a refinement of Lack’s principleAm. Nat. 100, 687–690 (doi:10.1086/282461[]
  • Wilson E. O. 1971The insect societies Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press []
  • Wilson E. O. 1974Sociobiology, the new synthesis Boston, MA: Harvard University Press []
  • Wilson D. S. 1980The natural selection of populations and communities Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin Cummings []
  • Wilson E. O., Bossert W. H. 1971A primer of population biology Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates []
  • Wilson D. S., Wilson E. O. 2007Rethinking the theoretical foundation of sociobiologyQuart. Rev. Biol. 82, 327–348 [PubMed[]
  • Wolf J. B., Brodie E. D., Moore A. J. 1999Interacting phenotypes and the evolutionary process. II. Selection resulting from social interactionsAm. Nat. 153, 254–266 (doi:10.1086/303168) [PubMed[]
  • Wolff J. O., Sherman P. W. 2007Rodent societies as model systems. In Rodent societies (eds Wolff J. O., Sherman P. W., editors. ). Chicago, IL: University Press []
  • Woolfenden G. E., Fitzpatrick J. W. 1984The Florida scrub jay: demography of a cooperative-breeding bird Princeton, NJ: University Press []
  • Wrangham R. W., Jones J. H., Laden G., Pilbeam D., Conklin-Brittain N. 1999The raw and the stolen—cooking and the ecology of human originsCurr. Anthropol. 40, 567–594 (doi:10.1086/300083) [PubMed[]
  • Zahavi A. 1975Mate selection: a selection for a handicapJ. Theoret. Biol. 53, 205–214 (doi:10.1016/0022-5193(75)90111-3) [PubMed[]
  • Zhou W.-X., Sornette D., Hill R. A., Dunbar R. 2004Discrete hierarchical organisation of social group sizesProc. R. Soc. Lond. B 272, 439–444 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2970[PMC free article] [PubMed[]

Leave a Reply

Please log in using one of these methods to post your comment:

WordPress.com Logo

You are commenting using your WordPress.com account. Log Out /  Change )

Twitter picture

You are commenting using your Twitter account. Log Out /  Change )

Facebook photo

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Log Out /  Change )

Connecting to %s